Tuesday, May 19, 2015

Suicide!



TORONTO ― Most people who die by suicide have no previous psychiatric diagnosis, new research shows.
A single-center study from California found that nearly two thirds of persons who completed suicide during a 3-year period had no established psychiatric diagnosis.
These findings suggest that "better detection of mental illness and treatment of at-risk patients may prevent completed suicides," said Nisha Ramsinghani, DO, from the Community Regional Medical Center, Fresno, California.
The findings also suggest that repeated suicide attempts are a "serious indicator of eventual successful suicide," she reported here at the American Psychiatric Association (APA) 2015 Annual Meeting.
"Our clinical observations suggested that the number of suicide attempts vary depending on the psychiatric diagnoses. We hypothesized that the number of previous attempts in completed suicides will also vary among different diagnoses," Dr Ramsinghani and her coauthors note in meeting materials.
To investigate, they analyzed the records of 205 people cared for at the Community Regional Medical Center in Fresno who died by suicide between 2010 and 2013.
Most of these individuals (n = 177, 85.9%) had no prior suicide attempt, and only 29 (14.1%) had one or more prior attempts. Women were more apt to have at least one prior attempt than men (26% vs 9%; P = .004). The mean age of the persons who completed suicide was 46.4 years.
Major depressive disorder was the most frequent diagnosis among those who completed suicide (21.8%), followed by substance use disorder (SUD) (14.1%), generalized anxiety disorder (4.9%), bipolar disorder (3.9%), schizophrenia (2.4%), borderline personality disorder (BPD) (1.9%), and posttraumatic stress disorder (1.9%).
It is noteworthy, Dr Ramsinghani said, that every person who completed suicide who had a diagnosis of BPD had at least one previous suicide attempt. Nearly two thirds of those who completed suicide and who had SUD also made at least one attempt before dying by suicide.
Interpret Cautiously
"Surprisingly," she said, in this cohort, 64.6% of persons who completed suicides had no prior (established) psychiatric diagnosis.
In an interview with Medscape Medical News, Mark Sinyor, MD, FRCPC, assistant professor, University of Toronto, and psychiatrist, Sunnybrook Health Sciences Centre, who was not involved in the study, cautioned that this was a single-center study, "and if you look at psychological autopsies that are done on suicides, more than 90% have a clear psychiatric diagnosis in retrospect."
"One of the issues is that even those people who can be identified as having a mental illness, not all of them have had recent contact with a healthcare provider or psychiatrist. I would hope that for everyone in the psychiatric community, suicide is top of mind," Dr Sinyor said.
It is also important to note that most people who die by suicide have high rates of other medical illnesses, "so all our colleagues in family medicine and other areas of medicine need heightened awareness of suicide as well and help make sure that people who show signs of being at risk get the appropriate care," Dr Sinyor said.
In addition, "as a society, we need to realize that many people have these thoughts, and people need to come get help. We need to educate the public and send the message that we have effective treatments for mental illness, life stressors, and trouble coping," Dr Sinyor said.

Tuesday, May 12, 2015

Pain is Pain!

 

Over-the-Counter Relief From Pains and Pleasures Alike

Acetaminophen Blunts Evaluation Sensitivity to Both Negative and Positive Stimuli

  1. Geoffrey R. O. Durso1
  2. Andrew Luttrell1
  3. Baldwin M. Way1,2
  1. 1Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University
  2. 2Institute for Behavioral Medicine Research, The Ohio State University Medical Center
  1. Geoffrey R. O. Durso, The Ohio State University, Department of Psychology, 1827 Neil Ave., Columbus, OH 43210 E-mail: durso.9@osu.edu
  2. Baldwin M. Way, The Ohio State University, Department of Psychology, 1827 Neil Ave., Columbus, OH 43210 E-mail: way.37@osu.edu
  1. Author Contributions G. R. O. Durso developed the study concept. All authors contributed to the study design. Data were collected and analyzed by G. R. O. Durso and A. Luttrell. G. R. O. Durso drafted the manuscript, and A. Luttrell and B. M. Way provided critical revisions. All authors approved the final version of the manuscript for submission.

Abstract

Acetaminophen, an effective and popular over-the-counter pain reliever (e.g., the active ingredient in Tylenol), has recently been shown to blunt individuals’ reactivity to a range of negative stimuli in addition to physical pain. Because accumulating research has shown that individuals’ reactivity to both negative and positive stimuli can be influenced by a single factor (an idea known as differential susceptibility), we conducted two experiments testing whether acetaminophen blunted individuals’ evaluations of and emotional reactions to both negative and positive images from the International Affective Picture System. Participants who took acetaminophen evaluated unpleasant stimuli less negatively and pleasant stimuli less positively, compared with participants who took a placebo. Participants in the acetaminophen condition also rated both negative and positive stimuli as less emotionally arousing than did participants in the placebo condition (Studies 1 and 2), whereas nonevaluative ratings (extent of color saturation in each image; Study 2) were not affected by drug condition. These findings suggest that acetaminophen has a general blunting effect on individuals’ evaluative and emotional processing, irrespective of negative or positive valence.

Wednesday, May 6, 2015


How our view of what makes us happy has changed in 80 years

Date:May 4, 2015
Source:British Psychological Society (BPS)
Summary:
Our view of what makes us happy has changed markedly since 1938. That is the conclusion of the psychologist who has recreated a famous study of happiness conducted in Bolton in 1938.
Quotations on happiness from the 1938 and 2014 surveys
"Enough money to meet everyday needs and a little for pleasure." (1938)
"Knowing that my rent is paid on time and I can afford to eat healthily." (2014)
"I would like a little home, not many possessions … congenial and satisfying companionship, the availability of good music and books." (1938)
"Engaging in my hobbies, spending time that is free of worry … Simple things like enjoying a nice meal or receiving care and affection." (2014)
"When I come home from the pit and see my kiddies and wife, I am happy." (1938)
"Simple things like going out for a walk…….you don't need tons of material things to be happy, you just have to be happy in the place you live and with the people around you." (2014)

Our view of what makes us happy has changed markedly since 1938. That is the conclusion of the psychologist Sandie McHugh from the Univeristy of Bolton who has recreated a famous study of happiness conducted in Bolton in 1938. She will present her study today, Tuesday 5 May 2015, to the Annual Conference of the British Psychological Society in Liverpool.
In 1938 Mass Observation placed an advertisement in the the Bolton Evening News asking readers to answer the question 'What is happiness?'. A total of 226 people sent letters in reply, and they were asked to help compile a happiness index by rating the importance of ten factors ranging from beauty to more security and religion.
In 2014 Sandie McHugh and Professor Jerome Carson repeated the Mass Observation survey by asking people from the town, via the Bolton News, to complete a questionnaire that repeated the questions from 1938 as closely was possible. She then compared the new findings with those from 1938.
Sandie McHugh found that in 1938 security, knowledge and religion were seen by participants as being the three most important aspects of happiness. In 2014 security was still in the top three, but good humour and leisure were in first and second places.
Religion, which was seen as the third most important factor in 1938, had fallen to tenth (and bottom) place in 1938.
Another striking difference is that in 1938 the majority of people said they were happiest when they were in Bolton, but in 2014 63 per cent said they were happier away from the town.
When it comes to luck, in 1938 and in 2014, 40 per cent of people believed it was important to happiness. In 2014, 77 per cent answered "No" to the question "Do you think your happiness is directly linked to material possessions and wealth?." Although security had been highly rated in 1938, wealth by itself was not.
Sandie McHugh said: "The overall impression from the correspondence in 1938 is that happiness factors were rooted in everyday lives at home and within the community. In 2014 many comments value family and friends, with good humour and leisure time also ranked highly."
Quotations on happiness from the 1938 and 2014 surveys
"Enough money to meet everyday needs and a little for pleasure." (1938)
"Knowing that my rent is paid on time and I can afford to eat healthily." (2014)
"I would like a little home, not many possessions … congenial and satisfying companionship, the availability of good music and books." (1938)
"Engaging in my hobbies, spending time that is free of worry … Simple things like enjoying a nice meal or receiving care and affection." (2014)
"When I come home from the pit and see my kiddies and wife, I am happy." (1938)
"Simple things like going out for a walk…….you don't need tons of material things to be happy, you just have to be happy in the place you live and with the people around you." (2014)

Story Source:
The above story is based on materials provided by British Psychological Society (BPS). Note: Materials may be edited for content and length.

 

Internet as new frontier in collecting data on the mind

Date:  April 30, 2015
Source:  Indiana University
Summary:
With Apple's launch of new health tracking tools for the iPhone and medical researchers' forays into Facebook to recruit clinical trial volunteers, Web and mobile apps are increasingly seen as a new source for health data. But psychologists are also looking to the Internet as a new source of information about the mind.
 
 
With Apple's launch of new health tracking tools for the iPhone and medical researchers' forays into Facebook to recruit clinical trial volunteers, Web and mobile apps are increasingly seen as a new source for health data.
But psychologists are also looking to the Internet as a new source of information about the mind -- and an Indiana University researcher is on the forefront of those developing the tools to make it happen.
Josh de Leeuw, a graduate student in the IU Bloomington College of Arts and Sciences' Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, is the creator of jsPsych, a free open-source software platform that employs a common Web technology to conduct psychology experiments over the Internet. The program allows psychologists without significant programming skills to deliver tasks common to research on the mind through a Web browser.
"Conducting psychology research online is appealing for a number of reasons: faster data collection, lower costs and improved anonymity of subjects and experimenters," de Leeuw said. "Internet users are more demographically diverse than the population conventionally sampled for behavioral research in psychology -- a fact that could contribute to results that better reflect the population at large."
This means the current pool of psychology research volunteers -- generally, college students -- could expand since participants would no longer need to travel to a researcher's lab to perform tasks common in psychological research. Using jsPsych, a psychologist could present anyone with Internet access with on-screen stimuli and then collect responses. Study instructions and consent forms could also be delivered electronically.
"Online experimentation is the bleeding edge of psychology research, and many believe online studies will play an important role in addressing the so-called 'replication crisis' in psychology," said Ben Motz, senior lecturer and director of undergraduate instruction in the Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, who has collaborated with de Leeuw. "Josh's work is at the epicenter of this -- and it's all the more impressive that he's developed this platform as a graduate student."
The replication crisis refers to a growing concern in experimental psychology -- and the larger scientific community -- about the drop in studies able to confirm previous work with experiments that achieve the same results using the same methods, as well as the increased risk of data manipulation in studies with small sample sizes.
Reducing costs and other barriers to psychological research could increase the number of replication studies, as well as the overall number of volunteers and data produced, in turn creating stronger results.
A few psychology researchers are already using online services to conduct research. One of those tools is Amazon Mechanical Turk, a service from the retail giant that enables anyone to hire online workers to perform short, simple tasks.
Others remain hesitant to employ these methods due to worries about the quality of data collected outside the highly controlled environment of a research lab. To address this concern, de Leeuw recently published a study in the journal Behavior Research Methods investigating whether one specific issue among brain researchers -- the potential delays in response introduced by the use of online technology -- represents a significant barrier to conducting research online.
That study looked at differences between Matlab's Psychophysics Toolbox -- one of the most popular software systems for psychological experiments -- and jsPsych.
To measure the speed that information is displayed on-screen and its effect on reaction time, de Leeuw asked 30 volunteers to sit in front of a projection screen and hold a thumb-activated switch in each hand. If volunteers saw the letter "N," they clicked a button with their dominant hand. If they did not see the letter, they clicked with their other hand. Volunteers repeated this experiment 400 times, with only a 45-second break in the middle.
To prevent weariness from affecting the results, de Leeuw set up the screen to project two images simultaneously -- one generated by jsPsych, the other by the traditional system -- enabling response times to both systems to be measured simultaneously.
In psychology research, response times may be used to discover which mental operations are more difficult than others, or to measure how uncertain people are about a decision. They can help distinguish between different theories of perception, memory, decision-making and social cognition. Psychologists refer to the study of the relationships between stimulus and response as "psychophysics." Measuring response times is only one of many applications of jsPsych.
The study revealed that jsPsych produced delays of 10 to 40 milliseconds compared to laboratory-based software. Three other statistical parameters measured showed no reliable difference between the systems.
"This is an extremely small lag time -- it's roughly the same as might be caused by other variations in experimental setup, such as a change in keyboard, mouse or monitor -- and it is constant between different experimental conditions," de Leeuw said. "In the context of a real human experiment, both systems are nearly identical."
He added that results are also applicable to other software packages that use JavaScript, the only programming language supported by all major Web browsers that does not require additional software to run.
"Hopefully this study will convince more people that you can, in fact, conduct psychophysics in a Web browser," de Leeuw said. "We're looking at technology with potential to transform how research on the brain gets done."
In addition to Motz, IU researchers contributing to the study were Richard Viken, Chris Eller, Michael Bailey, John Kruschke, Tony Walker and Alex Shroyer. Equipment and technical assistance were provided by the IU Advanced Visualization Laboratory. This research was supported by a National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship Grant (DGE-1342962).
A tutorial on jsPsych is available online. De Leeuw will also deliver a tutorial on the use of the program at the 2015 Cognitive Science Society meeting July 22.

Story Source:
The above story is based on materials provided by Indiana University. Note: Materials may be edited for content and length.